top of page
Search

Unpopular Philosophy of Psychology of Religion: Megacultism as a mental disorder.

Updated: Oct 12, 2022

Harvard neuroscientist Professor Robert Sapolsky regards megacultism (religion) as an evolved, adaptive response to pervasive stress. I agree. However, facts are often unusual things (and philosophers frequently debate their nature!), and sometimes facts that seem to be incommensurate go together in nature despite our disapproval and discomfort. There is little doubt that megacultism is a commonly deployed and exhibited evolved stress management device across all human societies. Does it really work? The truth is, of course, more complicated. It’s more complicated than most folk want it to be.



Overall, the stress management that Sapolsky touts doesn't really work in the sense that it does more harm than good. It works at best within limited scope and only on the surface of the psychology of the individual and social groups that adopt and deploy it. (It’s a more interesting fact than one might initially surmise that Sapolsky himself does not subscribe to it at all, and in fact outright rejects it as a method for his own stress management).


What's some of the hard social-psychological evidence it doesn't work? Megacult (religious) war. Subcult (Sectarian) violence. Abuse of women and unbelievers (apostates, heretics, and atheists). Pathological, maladaptive rejection of science and medicine. Proliferation of smaller cults. APD sufferers (psychopaths) who become religious leaders (the statistical prevalence of APD among these people is almost certainly as high as for CEOs) who hoodwink mentally sick adults with imaginary friends. People who drink animal urine. Homicides over nutty doctrines. Marital rape. There are hundreds of other outcomes - social, behavioural, group-behavioural, medical, and psycho-social, that demonstrate that it's really religious schizophrenia, and a pandemic thereof, that we are – as a civilisation - dealing with.


Theist-megacultism-based stress management doesn’t work because it’s based upon, and largely reduces to, maladaptive terror management for mortality and self-esteem, maladaptive emotion-driven re-appraisal in the face of thwarted goals, and lack of insight about the deployment of both. It's a maladaptive psychological response based upon emotion-driven re-appraisal in the face of goal-thwarting (S Folkman et al., 1986; Susan Folkman, 2013)

Megacultist (religious) theists are unequivocally mentally sick. They’re just sick from stress in different ways, and further sick from consequent maladaptive terror management (Arrowood & Cox, 2020; Castano et al., 2011; Friedman & Rholes, 2008; Jonas & Fischer, 2006; Pyszczynski et al., 2015; Vail et al., 2010, 2018).

In fact, one of the huge problems with Sapolsky’s conclusion about megacult-fideist stress-reduction is that it also doesn’t work because religious-theist schizophrenia induces more stress than it ‘manages’ by multiple mechanisms. Moreover, the additional induced stresses often don’t mitigate the allegedly managed or alleviated stresses, and in many cases just bury them under layers of psychosis and denial. This is of course – stressful. (Achtziger et al., 2015; Antelo et al., 2021; Babenko et al., 2018; R. F. Baumeister & Vohs, 2016b, 2016a; Brosschot et al., 2004; Buczny et al., 2015; Carnevali et al., 2020; Carpenter, 2019; Christiansen et al., 2012; Cohen & Wills, 1985; Dingcheng et al., 2018; Ellis & Del Giudice, 2019; Evans et al., 2016; Eysenck, 1985; Fischer et al., 2007, 2012; Fogaça & Duman, 2019; Susan Folkman, 2013; Ginty et al., 2012; Govorun & Payne, 2006; Gunthert et al., 1999; Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2016; Ihle et al., 2018; Kaiseler et al., 2009; Klein & McColl, 2019; Lange & Eggert, 2014; Lazarus, 1984; Leventhal et al., 1998; Liston et al., 2009; McEwen, 2012; McEwen & Sapolsky, 1995; Newman et al., 2018; Polman & Vohs, 2016; Reinecke et al., 2014; Rodrigues et al., 2009; Saldaña et al., 2021, 2022; Sapolsky, 2007; Schmeichel et al., 2003; Sharif, 2018; Simpson et al., 2006; Sjåstad & Baumeister, 2018; Sliwinski et al., 2006; Spada et al., 2008; Sturgeon et al., 2016; Taylor, 2006; Vohs & Zemack-Rugar, 2012; R. Wang et al., 2020; W. Wang et al., 2020; Xu et al., 2012; Zhu et al., 2019)

Megacult community membership doesn’t genuinely solve any of the problems of poverty, alienation, loneliness, ostracism, social stigma, group-stereotyping, relational aggression, victimisation by sociopaths and psychopaths, or domestic violence. It certainly doesn’t do so any more than membership in any non-religious or non-megacult group or community. In fact it’s well known that the doctrines, narratives, and beliefs of most theist megacults seriously exacerbate these problems for many people most of the time.


Theist megacultists’ partners still leave them for reasons of sexual, financial, and situational disappointment, and simple boredom. They must suffer the additional indignance, stress, and despondence associated with having to accept the outcome even when it demonstrates the frailty and impotence of their beliefs and the doctrine they have deployed in service of stress reduction. They are often forced to be consistently exposed to the emotionally abusive person in small community settings.


Theist megacultists are highly vulnerable to charismatic and ambitious cult leaders – and that includes ‘big four’ megacult leaders – who are anti-social psychopaths and abusive manipulators. (Of course every megacult leader with anti-social and narcissistic personality disorder will deny that this applies to them!). (Burkle, 2019; Fatfouta, 2019; Lee & Ashton, 2005; Miller et al., 2013; Rosenthal & Pittinsky, 2006)


Megacultists are often exposed to even more ostracism, alienation, and stigma than non-megacultists for perceived transgressions against megacult and religio-social codes. Defellowshipping (Mormons, Jehovah’s witnesses), ejection from community and home (Amish), murder of apostates (Islam), beating and rape of apostates (Islam), disenfranchisement and excommunication (Catholic), punishment and harm for breaching class boundaries (Hindu caste), bans on intermarriage, interference with personal and intimate relationships, inappropriate breaching of personal boundaries, and racial prejudices. The list is disparagingly enormous, and I have no room to continue it here.

Moreover there are many additional more complex and narratological stresses – what we might call metastresses – that come with megacult narratives, delusions, and membership. The believer is taught - badgered - to worry about what some supreme being thinks of them, and of their every behaviour. They’re made paranoid about being constantly watched, monitored, judged, and evaluated. This applies in many cases even to thoughts and mental imagery about perfectly natural behaviours like sex, which outcome is pathological according to almost all reasonable theories in health psychology.

Healthy conscience and self-evaluation are one thing. These outcomes are not that thing. Megacult theists have performance stress and the stress of keeping up appearances in the face of unusual and usually unreasonable and unrealistic moral and social demands. They must compete with other megacult members who may have many financial, social, educational, and situational advantages. Yet their ability to compete effectively is often intentionally deconstructed and impeded by the very doctrines they’re programmed and pressured to uphold and observe.


Then of course there’s living in constant fear of damnation, demons, superdemons (Satan), and the disapproval and punishment of their own best omnipotent friend, whom they further fear both disappointing and being separated from. None of this is mitigated much by salvation and scapegoat doctrines, which tend to induce further stresses associated with more paranoia and guilt about boundary crossing and prohibited doubts and thoughts.

Moreover, then there is the considerable cognitive dissonance involved in the continual suspension of disbelief. The stress of the basic cognitive dissonance of maintaining the delusional and dissimulative belief system in the face of evolved ratiocination and information tracking that’s designed to penetrate it is a paradigm case study for ego-depletion theory (Roy F. Baumeister, 2002; Roy F. Baumeister et al., 1998, 2007; Blázquez et al., 2017; Burkley, 2008; Ciarocco et al., 2001; Fischer et al., 2007, 2008; Hagger et al., 2016; Inzlicht & Friese, 2019; Moller et al., 2006; Polman & Vohs, 2016; Ren et al., 2010; Schmeichel et al., 2006; Xiao et al., 2014; Xu et al., 2012; Yam et al., 2014).

Let's be real and bite the bullet. Sapolsky’s really saying devout religious theism is schizophrenia. He's not risking his tenure to do it openly without plausible deniability, but: get the hint. Even if he’s not saying it, he’s effectively saying it.

Journals and e-magazines like Open Culture are not going to risk losing readership with uncomfortable truths/facts either. Look what happened to Richard Dawkins.

Evolved truth tracking and - more importantly - evolved information tracking are maladaptive and deranged in the religious schizophrenic. So is their emotion-driven re-appraisal in relation to thwarted goals and their lack of insight into their disorder.

Sapolsky is fairly smart, and so he doesn't want to annoy religious schizophrenics and the often psychopathic handlers who profit from them. He doesn’t want to upset the complex web of cultural narratives which govern both the artifices and foundations of academic tenure. He’s not disingenuous or lacking in integrity. He’s just not reckless with his own house.

There are so many religious schizophrenics, and so many others that depend upon, or at least believe they depend upon, the perpetuation of the status quo, that facing down the pandemic of religious schizophrenia is daunting at best. Nonetheless, huge numbers of people have heart disease and cancer: but we don't stop calling those disease states. Even more people have religious-theist schizophrenia.

Sapolsky wants to say it. He's effectively saying it. He's got too much at stake:




The solid literature supporting the stress-reducing benefits of megacultism that Sapolsky refers to is almost certainly - in most cases - affected by the worst aspects of the reproducibility crisis and its foundations in p-hacking, HARKing, publication bias, confirmation bias, and non-fitting-data bin-filing (Bird, 2020; Brian Resnick, 2017; Lurquin & Miyake, 2017; Munafò et al., 2022; Schooler, 2014; Trafimow & Earp, 2016). More relevant, however, are the errors in premises, the cultural biases, and other kinds of problems that are better captured by what has been called the theory crisis in psychological science. This crisis is about the problems of operationalisation of concepts into variables and the premises and grounding thereof (Eronen & Bringmann, 2021). The entire approach to the question of megacultism is constrained by cultural narratives and expectations that are so pervasive and influential as to be considered normative guides to normative approaches to evaluating megacultism.


Well-tracking the information of cultural and social trends and dynamics in one’s local community by deploying evolved cognitive truth-tracking and information-tracking only provides a partial basis for mental health. The individual must also be able to reconcile into their ratiocination, on an intuitive and heuristic normative basis, the scientific episteme that's now widely accessible to all people, and to resist admitting exactly the kind of anti-reasoning (against epistemic rationality) that Sapolsky notes is prevalent in theist fideism(Ståhl & van Prooijen, 2018, 2021; Thurow, 2013; Van Der Hoek, 2005).

Put in other terms, when one appraises the data properly and without inappropriate cultural and confirmatory biases there are no meaningful or significant distinguishing psychological differences – including from the perspective of dimensional diagnostic criteria - between schizophrenia and the psychological state of adults having imaginary friends, paranoid delusions about demons, and delusions of reference centred on their imaginary power-friends (and that’s only a few of the dimensional diagnostic criteria).

I once had a schizophrenia patient as an intimate partner, and they were diagnosed significantly on the basis that they heard voices speaking to them. But they were also committed (by their doctor without my knowledge) on the basis that they thought that if a street sign was a particular colour, or some businessperson in the street was holding their briefcase in their right hand: this was a directive from the universe to return to China. How is this different from people believing they are being guided by signs in various forms such as religious texts and miracles? It isn’t. In fact my friend was significantly less ill than religious schizophrenics because she did not have an imaginary friend whom she thought responsible for the messages!

The stress-relieving benefits of megacultism-based psychological placebos for terror management are likely to be far more limited than it is commonly popular to believe and endorse. Perhaps their endorsement is yet another terror-management strategy for warding off ego-depletion and terror of mortality, loss of self-esteem, and of course loss of career, social status, and reputation! The religious-theistic schizophrenia pandemic is its own catalyst. Just as the infected dead bodies in a plague proliferate the deadly viral or bacterial pathogens that case the plague, the afflicted in the religious schizophrenia pandemic proliferate the memetic and narratological misinformation and disinformation that causes and exacerbates the religious schizophrenia pandemic (Castelfranchi, 2001). Others use the infected for their own purposes (tossing bodies over the castle wall or handing infected towels to the native tribespeople?), and the rest of us look on in subdued terror.


References


Achtziger, A., Alós-Ferrer, C., & Wagner, A. K. (2015). Money, depletion, and prosociality in the dictator game. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics. https://doi.org/10.1037/npe0000031


Antelo, E., Saldaña, O., & Rodríguez-Carballeira, Á. (2021). The impact of group psychological abuse on distress: the mediating role of social functioning and resilience. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 12(1). https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2021.1954776


Arrowood, R. B., & Cox, C. R. (2020). Terror management theory: A practical review of research and application. Brill Research Perspectives in Religion and …. https://brill.com/view/journals/rpsy/2/1/article-p1_1.xml


Babenko, V. N., Smagin, D. A., Galyamina, A. G., Kovalenko, I. L., & Kudryavtseva, N. N. (2018). Altered Slc25 family gene expression as markers of mitochondrial dysfunction in brain regions under experimental mixed anxiety/depression-like disorder. BMC Neuroscience, 19(1), 79–79. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12868-018-0480-6


Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2016b). Strength model of self-regulation as limited resource: Assessment, controversies, update. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2016.04.001


Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2016a). Strength model of self-regulation as limited resource: Assessment, controversies, update. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 54, pp. 67–127). Academic Press Inc. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2016.04.001


Baumeister, Roy F. (2002). Ego Depletion and Self-Control Failure: An Energy Model of the Self’s Executive Function. Self and Identity, 1(2), 129–136. https://doi.org/10.1080/152988602317319302


Baumeister, Roy F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252


Baumeister, Roy F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 351–355. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x


Bird, A. (2020). Understanding the Replication Crisis as a Base Rate Fallacy. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 000–000. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjps/axy051


Blázquez, D., Botella, J., & Suero, M. (2017). The debate on the ego-depletion effect: Evidence from meta-analysis with the p-uniform method. In Frontiers in Psychology (Vol. 8, Issue FEB). Frontiers Research Foundation. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00197


Brian Resnick. (2017). What a nerdy debate about p-values shows about science — and how to fix it. Vox.Com.


Brosschot, J. F., Van Dijk, E., & Thayer, J. F. (2004). Daily episodes of worrying and stressors increase daytime- and nighttime cardiac activity. Psychology and Health, 19(SUPPL. 1), 29. https://doi.org/10.1080/0887044031000141199


Buczny, J., Layton, R. L., & Muraven, M. (2015). The role of implicit affective responses and trait self-control in ego resource management. Motivation and Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-015-9489-7


Burkle, F. M. (2019). Character disorders among autocratic world leaders and the impact on health security, human rights, and humanitarian care. Prehospital and Disaster Medicine, 34(1), 2–7. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1049023X18001280


Burkley, E. (2008). The role of self-control in resistance to persuasion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(3), 419–431. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207310458


Carnevali, L., Montano, N., Tobaldini, E., Thayer, J. F., & Sgoifo, A. (2020). The contagion of social defeat stress: Insights from rodent studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 111, 12–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.011


Carpenter, C. J. (2019). Cognitive dissonance, ego-involvement, and motivated reasoning. Annals of the International Communication Association, 43(1), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2018.1564881


Castano, E., Leidner, B., Bonacossa, A., Nikkah, J., Perrulli, R., Spencer, B., & Humphrey, N. (2011). Ideology, Fear of Death, and Death Anxiety. Political Psychology, 32(4), 601–621. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2011.00822.x


Castelfranchi, C. (2001). Towards a cognitive memetics: Socio-cognitive mechanisms for memes selection and spreading. Journal of Memetics-Evolutionary Models of Information Transmission, 5(section 6), 1–14. http://cfpm.org/jom-emit/2001/vol5/castelfranchi_c.html


Christiansen, P., Cole, J. C., & Field, M. (2012). Ego depletion increases Ad-Lib alcohol consumption: Investigating cognitive mediators and moderators. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026623


Ciarocco, N. J., Sommer, K. L., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Ostracism and ego depletion: The strains of silence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(9), 1156–1163. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201279008


Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, Social Support, and the Buffering Hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98(2), 310–357. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310


Dingcheng, R., Chenhong, H., & Tianjia, C. (2018). Physical and mental harm caused by participation in cults from a psychological perspective. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(12), 2091–2100. http://journal.psych.ac.cn/xlkxjz/EN/Y2018/V26/I12/2091#1


Ellis, B. J., & Del Giudice, M. (2019). Developmental Adaptation to Stress: An Evolutionary Perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 70(1), 111–139. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011732


Evans, D. R., Boggero, I. A., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2016). The Nature of Self-Regulatory Fatigue and “Ego Depletion”: Lessons From Physical Fatigue. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(4), 291–310. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868315597841


Eysenck, H. J. (1985). Stress, appraisal, and coping. Behaviour Research and Therapy. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(85)90087-7


Fatfouta, R. (2019). Facets of narcissism and leadership: A tale of Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde? Human Resource Management Review, 29(4). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2018.10.002


Fischer, P., Greitemeyer, T., & Frey, D. (2007). Ego depletion and positive illusions: Does the construction of positivity require regulatory resources? In Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin (Vol. 33, Issue 9, pp. 1306–1321). https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207303025


Fischer, P., Greitemeyer, T., & Frey, D. (2008). Self-Regulation and Selective Exposure: The Impact of Depleted Self-Regulation Resources on Confirmatory Information Processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.3.382


Fischer, P., Kastenmüller, A., & Asal, K. (2012). Ego depletion increases risk-taking. Journal of Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2012.683894


Fogaça, M. V., & Duman, R. S. (2019). Cortical GABAergic dysfunction in stress and depression: New insights for therapeutic interventions. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 13, 87–87. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2019.00087


Folkman, S, Lazarus, R., psychology, C. D.-S.-… social, & 1986, undefined. (1986). Dynamics of a stressful encounter: cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes. Psycnet.Apa.OrgSign In. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.992


Folkman, Susan. (2013). Stress: Appraisal and Coping. In Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1005-9_215


Friedman, M., & Rholes, W. S. (2008). Religious fundamentalism and terror management. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 18(1), 36–52. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508610701719322


Ginty, A. T., Phillips, A. C., Roseboom, T. J., Carroll, D., & DeRooij, S. R. (2012). Cardiovascular and cortisol reactions to acute psychological stress and cognitive ability in the Dutch Famine Birth Cohort Study. Psychophysiology, 49(3), 391–400. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1469-8986.2011.01316.X


Govorun, O., & Payne, B. K. (2006). Ego-depletion and prejudice: Separating automatic and controlled components. Social Cognition. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2006.24.2.111


Gunthert, K. C., Cohen, L. H., & Armeli, S. (1999). The role of neuroticism in daily stress and coping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1087


Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2016). Commentary: Misguided effort with elusive implications, and sifting signal from noise with replication science. In Frontiers in Psychology (Vol. 7, Issue APR). Frontiers Research Foundation. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00621


Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Alberts, H., Anggono, C. O., Batailler, C., Birt, A. R., Brand, R., Brandt, M. J., Brewer, G. A., Bruyneel, S., Calvillo, D. P., Campbell, W. K., Cannon, P. R., Carlucci, M., Carruth, N. P., Cheung, T. T. L., Crowell, A., De Ridder, D. T. D., Dewitte, S., … Zwienenberg, M. (2016). A Multilab Preregistered Replication of the Ego-Depletion Effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 546–573. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616652873


Ihle, A., Oris, M., Sauter, J., Rimmele, U., & Kliegel, M. (2018). Cognitive reserve and social capital accrued in early and midlife moderate the relation of psychological stress to cognitive performance in old age. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 45(3–4), 190–197. https://doi.org/10.1159/000488052


Inzlicht, M., & Friese, M. (2019). The Past, Present, and Future of Ego Depletion. In Social Psychology (Vol. 50, Issues 5–6, pp. 370–378). Hogrefe Publishing GmbH. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000398


Jonas, E., & Fischer, P. (2006). Terror management and religion: Evidence that intrinsic religiousness mitigates worldview defense following mortality salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(3), 553–567. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.553


Kaiseler, M., Polman, R., & Nicholls, A. (2009). Mental toughness, stress, stress appraisal, coping and coping effectiveness in sport. Personality and Individual Differences. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.012


Klein, J., & McColl, G. (2019). Cognitive dissonance: how self-protective distortions can undermine clinical judgement. Medical Education, 53(12), 1178–1186. https://doi.org/10.1111/medu.13938


Lange, F., & Eggert, F. (2014). Sweet delusion. Glucose drinks fail to counteract ego depletion. Appetite, 75, 54–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.12.020


Lazarus, R. S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal, and Coping - Richard S. Lazarus, PhD, Susan Folkman, PhD - Google Books. In Health Psychology: A Handbook.


Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2005). Psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and Narcissism in the Five-Factor Model and the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(7), 1571–1582. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.016


Leventhal, H., Patrick-Miller, L., & Leventhal, E. A. (1998). It’s long-term stressors that take a toll: Comment on Cohen et al. (1998). Health Psychology, 17(3), 211–213. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.17.3.211


Liston, C., McEwen, B. S., & Casey, B. J. (2009). Psychosocial stress reversibly disrupts prefrontal processing and attentional control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(3), 912–917. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0807041106


Lurquin, J. H., & Miyake, A. (2017). Challenges to ego-depletion research go beyond the replication crisis: A need for tackling the conceptual crisis. In Frontiers in Psychology (Vol. 8, Issue APR). Frontiers Research Foundation. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00568


McEwen, B. S. (2012). Brain on stress: How the social environment gets under the skin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(SUPPL.2), 17180–17185. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1121254109


McEwen, B. S., & Sapolsky, R. M. (1995). Stress and cognitive function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5(2), 205–216. https://doi.org/10.1016/0959-4388(95)80028-X


Miller, J. D., Gentile, B., Wilson, L., & Campbell, W. K. (2013). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism and the DSM-5 pathological personality trait model. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95(3), 284–290. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2012.685907


Moller, A. C., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006). Choice and ego-depletion: The moderating role of autonomy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206288008


Munafò, M. R., Chambers, C., Collins, A., Fortunato, L., & Macleod, M. (2022). The reproducibility debate is an opportunity, not a crisis. BMC Research Notes, 15(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/S13104-022-05942-3


Newman, E. L., Leonard, M. Z., Arena, D. T., de Almeida, R. M. M., & Miczek, K. A. (2018). Social defeat stress and escalation of cocaine and alcohol consumption: Focus on CRF. Neurobiology of Stress, 9, 151–165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2018.09.007


Polman, E., & Vohs, K. D. (2016). Decision fatigue, choosing for others, and self-construal. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(5), 471–478. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616639648


Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Greenberg, J. (2015). Thirty Years of Terror Management Theory: From Genesis to Revelation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 1–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.03.001


Reinecke, L., Hartmann, T., & Eden, A. (2014). The guilty couch potato: The role of ego depletion in reducing recovery through media use. Journal of Communication. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12107


Ren, J., Hu, L., Zhang, H., & Huang, Z. (2010). Implicit positive emotion counteracts ego depletion. Social Behavior and Personality, 38(7), 919–928. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2010.38.7.919


Rodrigues, S. M., LeDoux, J. E., & Sapolsky, R. M. (2009). The influence of stress hormones on fear circuitry. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 289–313. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135620


Rosenthal, S. A., & Pittinsky, T. L. (2006). Narcissistic leadership. Leadership Quarterly, 17(6), 617–633. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2006.10.005


Saldaña, O., Rodríguez-Carballeira, Á., Almendros, C., & Guilera, G. (2021). Group Psychological Abuse and Psychopathological Symptoms: The Mediating Role of Psychological Stress. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 36(11–12), NP6602–NP6623. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260518815710


Saldaña, O., Wu-Salmeron, O., Antelo, E., & Rodríguez-Carballeira, Á. (2022). The Negative Impact of Group Psychological Abuse on Life Satisfaction and Well-being. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 37(19–20), NP18865–NP18887. https://doi.org/10.1177/08862605211042598


Sapolsky, R. (2007). Stress, stress-related disease, and emotional regulation. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2007-01392-030


Schmeichel, B. J., Demaree, H. A., Robinson, J. L., & Pu, J. (2006). Ego depletion by response exaggeration. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.02.005


Schmeichel, B. J., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Intellectual Performance and Ego Depletion: Role of the Self in Logical Reasoning and Other Information Processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.33


Schooler, J. W. (2014). Metascience could rescue the “replication crisis.” In Nature. https://doi.org/10.1038/515009a


Sharif, A. (2018). Effect of Stress on Academic Performance of Undergraduate Medical Students. http://159.203.91.186/handle/123456789/722


Simpson, B., Irwin, K., & Lawrence, P. (2006). Does a “norm of self-interest” discourage prosocial behavior? Rationality and quid pro quo in charitable giving. Social Psychology Quarterly, 69(3), 296–306. https://doi.org/10.1177/019027250606900306


Sjåstad, H., & Baumeister, R. F. (2018). The Future and the Will: Planning requires self-control, and ego depletion leads to planning aversion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 76, 127–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.01.005


Sliwinski, M. J., Smyth, J. M., Hofer, S. M., & Stawski, R. S. (2006). Intraindividual coupling of daily stress and cognition. Psychology and Aging, 21(3), 545–557. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.545


Spada, M. M., Nikčević, A. V., Moneta, G. B., & Wells, A. (2008). Metacognition, perceived stress, and negative emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(5), 1172–1181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.010


Ståhl, T., & van Prooijen, J. W. (2018). Epistemic rationality: Skepticism toward unfounded beliefs requires sufficient cognitive ability and motivation to be rational. Personality and Individual Differences, 122, 155–163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.10.026


Ståhl, T., & van Prooijen, J. W. (2021). Analytic atheism: Valuing epistemic rationality strengthens the association between analytic thinking and religious disbelief. Personality and Individual Differences, 179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.110914


Sturgeon, J. A., Arewasikporn, A., Okun, M. A., Davis, M. C., Ong, A. D., & Zautra, A. J. (2016). The psychosocial context of financial stress: Implications for inflammation and psychological health. Psychosomatic Medicine, 78(2), 134–143. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000276


Taylor, S. E. (2006). Tend and befriend: Biobehavioral bases of affiliation under stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 273–277. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00451.x


Thurow, J. C. (2013). Does cognitive science show belief in god to be irrational? The epistemic consequences of the cognitive science of religion. International Journal for Philosophy of Religion, 74(1), 77–98. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11153-011-9300-y


Trafimow, D., & Earp, B. D. (2016). Badly specified theories are not responsible for the replication crisis in social psychology: Comment on Klein. Theory and Psychology, 26(4), 540–548. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354316637136


Vail, K. E., Rothschild, Z. K., Weise, D. R., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (2010). A terror management analysis of the psychological functions of religion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(1), 84–94. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309351165


Vail, K. E., Soenke, M., & Waggoner, B. (2018). Terror management theory and religious belief. Handbook of Terror Management Theory, 259–285. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-811844-3.00011-1


Van Der Hoek, W. (2005). Information, interaction and agency. In Information, Interaction, and Agency. Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/1-4020-4094-6


Vohs, K., & Zemack-Rugar, Y. (2012). Deepening our understanding of depletion: New causes, boundaries, and processes. Advances in Consumer Research.


Wang, R., He, Y., Xu, J., & Zhang, H. (2020). Fake news or bad news? Toward an emotion-driven cognitive dissonance model of misinformation diffusion. Asian Journal of Communication, 30(5), 317–342. https://doi.org/10.1080/01292986.2020.1811737


Wang, W., Liu, W., Duan, D., Bai, H., Wang, Z., & Xing, Y. (2020). Chronic social defeat stress mouse model: Current view on its behavioral deficits and modifications. Behavioral Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000418


Xiao, S., Dang, J., Mao, L., & Liljedahl, S. (2014). When more depletion offsets the ego depletion effect. Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000197


Xu, H., Bègue, L., & Bushman, B. J. (2012). Too fatigued to care: Ego depletion, guilt, and prosocial behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(5), 1183–1186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.03.007


Yam, K. C., Chen, X. P., & Reynolds, S. J. (2014). Ego depletion and its paradoxical effects on ethical decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.03.008


Zhu, Y., Klomparens, E. A., Guo, S., & Geng, X. (2019). Neuroinflammation caused by mental stress: the effect of chronic restraint stress and acute repeated social defeat stress in mice. Neurological Research, 41(8), 762–769. https://doi.org/10.1080/01616412.2019.1615670








120 views0 comments
bottom of page